💾 Archived View for gmi.noulin.net › mobileNews › 5860.gmi captured on 2024-03-21 at 18:10:15. Gemini links have been rewritten to link to archived content

View Raw

More Information

⬅️ Previous capture (2023-01-29)

➡️ Next capture (2024-05-10)

-=-=-=-=-=-=-

Don t Make Important Decisions Late in the Day

2016-02-25 11:03:41

Francesca Gino

February 23, 2016

You arrive at the office and, as usual, face a long list of tasks to

accomplish: getting back to your colleagues about joint projects, sending those

time-sensitive e-mails, finishing up that important performance review, making

progress on one project, deciding on next steps for another. What s the best

way to tackle your to-do list?

You may have devised a personal strategy to help you order your various tasks.

But if you are like most people, you may not give too much thought about when

is the best time to tackle each of them. You should. Recent research suggests

that you need to think more strategically about how the time of day affects

your decisions and performance.

Over the course of a regular day, everyone s mental resources get taxed,

research has consistently shown. Thus, as the day wears on, whether you like it

or not, you become increasingly fatigued and consequently more likely to

underperform on work tasks. Cognitive fatigue in a very common condition that

results from sustained engagement that taxes your mental resources. This seems

obvious, right? Yet the vast majority of people often overlook cognitive

fatigue, despite the fact that it influences their choices and behaviors in

profound ways.

Research has found that persistent cognitive fatigue results in burnout at

work, lower motivation, increased distractibility, and poor information

processing. It even lowers the quality of everyone s judgment and decisions,

including those of experts.

For instance, scientists Shai Danziger, Jonathan Levav, and Liora Avnaim-Pesso

analyzed 1,112 bench rulings in a parole court and plotted the proportion of

favorable rulings over the course of the day. They found that judges were more

likely to deny a prisoner s request and accept the status quo outcome as they

advanced through the sequence of cases on a given day. More specifically, their

proportion of favorable rulings started out high, at about 65% at the start of

the day, but dropped off rapidly.

By the time a meal break came around, the proportion of favorable rulings was

close to zero. When court was back in session, the pattern repeated itself,

starting high and ending with almost zero favorable rulings. Interestingly,

neither the judges nor the panelists who advised them were aware that mental

energy is essential to careful deliberation and the later in the day, the lower

such energy is, unless a lunch break allows judges to recharge.

Evidence for the same type of cognitive fatigue has been found in other

contexts, including consumers choosing among various products and physicians

prescribing antibiotics. Primary care doctors often prescribe unnecessary

antibiotics for acute respiratory infections (ARI), researchers have found. As

the physicians appeared to wear down during their morning and afternoon

clinic sessions, the rates at which they prescribed antibiotics increased.

About 5% more patients receive antibiotics at the end of a clinic session as

compared to the beginning, this research shows. Thus, while clinicians make

many patient care decisions each day, the cumulative demand of these decisions

leads to more inappropriate choices later in the day.

Considered together, this evidence points to a clear conclusion: The overall

demand of multiple decisions on people s cognitive resources throughout the day

erodes their ability to resist making easier and potentially inappropriate or

bad decisions.

Similarly, time of day can affect performance. Hans Henrik Sievertsen of the

Danish National Centre for Social Research, Marco Piovesan of the University of

Copenhagen, and I found that time of day affects students performance in

schools. Using test data on the full population of 8- to 15-year-old children

in Danish public schools from school years 2009/10 and 2012/13 a sample of

over two million data points we measured the effect of both time of day and

breaks on students performance on standardized tests. Consistent with our

predictions, cognitive fatigue led students to perform worse on the tests, and

breaks recharged their energy.

Specifically, our analyses led to three main findings:

The later in the day the time of the test was, the lower students performance

on the test

Breaks caused a significant improvement in performance

The effect of time of day and of breaks was not homogeneous that is,

low-performing students were more affected by breaks (and also by the time of

the day when the test was taken) than high-performing students

Importantly, a 20- to 30-minute break causes an improvement in test scores that

is larger than an hour s deterioration. So, if there were a break after every

hour, test scores would actually improve over the course of the day. But if

breaks occur only every other hour like they do in the Danish system, the total

effect is negative.

As this research on experts and students suggests, time of day and breaks have

a meaningful influence on your decisions, behavior, and performance. So as you

think through your to-do list, you may want to carefully consider the role of

these external factors. Make sure you tackle the tasks that require a great

deal of attention and mental energy earlier in the day and take breaks

throughout the day.

Francesca Gino is a professor at Harvard Business School, a faculty affiliate

of the Behavioral Insights Group at Harvard Kennedy School, and the author of

Sidetracked: Why Our Decisions Get Derailed, and How We Can Stick to the Plan

(Harvard Business Review Press, 2013). She cochairs an HBS executive education

program on applying behavioral economics to organizational problems. Twitter:

@francescagino.