💾 Archived View for gmi.noulin.net › mobileNews › 1125.gmi captured on 2023-12-28 at 20:24:15. Gemini links have been rewritten to link to archived content
⬅️ Previous capture (2023-01-29)
-=-=-=-=-=-=-
2009-03-25 14:52:20
LiveScience.com livescience Staff
livescience.com Tue Mar 24, 10:45 pm ET
Are you listening to me? Didn't I just tell you to get your coat? Helloooo!
It's cold out there...
So goes many a conversation between parent and toddler. It seems everything you
tell them either falls on deaf ears or goes in one ear and out the other. But
that's not how it works.
Toddlers listen, they just store the information for later use, a new study
finds.
"I went into this study expecting a completely different set of findings," said
psychology professor Yuko Munakata at the University of Colorado at Boulder.
"There is a lot of work in the field of cognitive development that focuses on
how kids are basically little versions of adults trying to do the same things
adults do, but they're just not as good at it yet. What we show here is they
are doing something completely different."
Munakata and colleagues used a computer game and a setup that measures the
diameter of the pupil of the eye to determine the mental effort of the child to
study the cognitive abilities of 3-and-a-half-year-olds and 8-year-olds.
The game involved teaching children simple rules about two cartoon characters -
Blue from Blue's Clues and SpongeBob SquarePants - and their preferences for
different objects. The children were told that Blue likes watermelon, so they
were to press the happy face on the computer screen only when they saw Blue
followed by a watermelon. When SpongeBob appeared, they were to press the sad
face on the screen.
"The older kids found this sequence easy, because they can anticipate the
answer before the object appears," said doctoral student Christopher Chatham,
who participated in the study. "But preschoolers fail to anticipate in this
way. Instead, they slow down and exert mental effort after being presented with
the watermelon, as if they're thinking back to the character they had seen only
after the fact."
The pupil measurements showed that 3-year-olds neither plan for the future nor
live completely in the present. Instead, they call up the past as they need it.
"For example, let's say it's cold outside and you tell your 3-year-old to go
get his jacket out of his bedroom and get ready to go outside," Chatham
explained. "You might expect the child to plan for the future, think 'OK it's
cold outside so the jacket will keep me warm.' But what we suggest is that this
isn't what goes on in a 3-year-old's brain. Rather, they run outside, discover
that it is cold, and then retrieve the memory of where their jacket is, and
then they go get it."
The findings are detailed this week in the Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences.
Munakata figures the results might help with real situations.
"If you just repeat something again and again that requires your young child to
prepare for something in advance, that is not likely to be effective," Munakata
said. "What would be more effective would be to somehow try to trigger this
reactive function. So don't do something that requires them to plan ahead in
their mind, but rather try to highlight the conflict that they are going to
face. Perhaps you could say something like 'I know you don't want to take your
coat now, but when you're standing in the yard shivering later, remember that
you can get your coat from your bedroom."