💾 Archived View for gmi.noulin.net › mobileNews › 5390.gmi captured on 2023-09-08 at 17:43:50. Gemini links have been rewritten to link to archived content

View Raw

More Information

⬅️ Previous capture (2023-01-29)

➡️ Next capture (2024-05-10)

-=-=-=-=-=-=-

Social loafing

2015-04-30 12:01:54

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

In the social psychology of groups, social loafing is the phenomenon of people

exerting less effort to achieve a goal when they work in a group than when they

work alone.[1][2] This is seen as one of the main reasons groups are sometimes

less productive than the combined performance of their members working as

individuals, but should be distinguished from the accidental coordination

problems that groups sometimes experience.[citation needed]

Social loafing can be explained by the "free-rider" theory and the resulting

"sucker effect", which is an individual s reduction in effort in order to avoid

pulling the weight of a fellow group member.[3][4] Research on social loafing

began with rope pulling experiments by Ringelmann, who found that members of a

group tended to exert less effort in pulling a rope than did individuals alone.

In more recent research, studies involving modern technology, such as online

and distributed groups, have also shown clear evidence of social loafing. Many

of the causes of social loafing stem from an individual feeling that his or her

effort will not matter to the group.

History

Rope-pulling experiments

Results from Ringelmann's experiment

The first known research on the social loafing effect began in 1913 with Max

Ringelmann's study. He found that when he asked a group of men to pull on a

rope, that they did not pull as hard collectively as they did when each was

pulling alone. This research did not distinguish whether this was the result of

the individuals in a group putting in less effort or of poor coordination

within the group.[5][6] In 1974, Alan Ingham and colleagues replicated

Ringelmann's experiment using two types of group: 1) Groups with real

participants in groups of various sizes (consistent with Ringelmann's setup) or

2) Pseudo-groups with only one real participant. In the pseudo-groups, the

researchers' assistants only pretended to pull on the rope. The results showed

a decrease in the participants' performance, with groups of participants who

all exerted effort suffering the largest declines. Because the pseudo-groups

were isolated from coordination effects (since the participant's confederates

did not physically pull the rope), Ingham proved that communication alone did

not account for the effort decrease, and that motivational losses were the more

likely cause of the performance decline.[7]

Clapping and shouting experiments

In contrast with Ringelmann's first findings, Bibb Latan et al. replicated

previous social loafing findings while demonstrating that the decreased

performance of groups was attributable to reduced individual effort, as

distinct from a deterioration due to coordination. They showed this by

blindfolding male college students while making them wear headphones that

masked all noise. They then asked them to shout both in actual groups and

pseudogroups in which they shouted alone but believed they were shouting with

others. When subjects believed one other person was shouting, they shouted 82%

as intensely as they did alone, but with five others, their effort decreased to

74%.

Latan et al. concluded that increasing the number of people in a group

diminished the relative social pressure on each person: "If the individual

inputs are not identifiable the person may work less hard. Thus if the person

is dividing up the work to be performed or the amount of reward he expects to

receive, he will work less hard in groups."[8][9]

Meta-analysis study and the Collective Effort Model (CEM)

In a 1993 meta-analysis by Karau and Williams, they propose the Collective

Effort Model (CEM), which is used to generate predictions.[1] The CEM

integrates expectancy theories with theories of group-level social comparison

and social identity to account for studies that examine individual effort in

collective settings. From a psychological state, it proposes that Expectancy

multiplied by Instrumentality multiplied by Valence of Outcome produces the

resulting Motivational Force.

Karau et al.'s concluded that social loafing occurred because there was usually

a stronger perceived contingency between individual effort and valued outcomes

when working individually. When working collectively, other factors frequently

determine performance, and valued outcomes are also divided among all group

members. All individuals are assumed to try to maximize the expected utility of

their actions. The CEM also acknowledges that some valued outcomes do not

depend on performance. For example, exerting strong effort when working on

intrinsically meaningful tasks or with highly respected team members may result

in self-satisfaction or approval from the group, even if the high effort had

little to no impact on tangible performance outcomes.[1]

Notable or novel findings by Karau and Williams following their implementation

of the CEM include:

The magnitude of social loafing is reduced for women and individuals

originating from Eastern cultures.

Individuals are more likely to loaf when their co-workers are expected to

perform well.

Individuals reduce social loafing when working with acquaintances and do not

loaf at all when they work in highly valued groups.[1]

Dispersed versus collocated groups

A 2005 study by Laku Chidambaram and Lai Lai Tung based their research model on

Latan s social impact theory, and hypothesized that as group size and

dispersion grew, the group s work would be affected in the following areas:

Members would contribute less in both quantity and quality, final group output

would be of lower quality, and a group s output would be affected both by

individual factors and contextual factors.

A sample of 240 undergraduate business students was randomly split into forty

teams (half of the teams were 4-person and half 8-person) which were randomly

assigned to either a collocated or distributed setting. The participants were

to complete a task that asked them to act as a board of directors of a winery

with an image problem. They were to find and discuss alternatives, and at the

end submit their alternative with rationale. Collocated groups worked at a

table together, while distributed groups did the same task at separate

computers that allowed for electronic, networked communication. The same

technology was used by both collocated and distributed groups.

Chidambaram and Tung found that group size mattered immensely in a group s

performance. The smaller the group, the more likely each member was to

participate, regardless of range (dispersed or collocated). The main difference

stated between distributed and collocated groups was the social pressure at

least to appear busy that is present in collocated groups. When others are

present, people feel the need to look as if they are working hard, while those

who are not in the presence of others do not.[10]

Effect of culture

In 1989, Christopher P. Earley hypothesized that social loafing would be

mitigated in collectivist cultures that focused more on achievement of the

group than the individual. He conducted a study in the United States and China,

two polar opposites in terms of culture (with the U.S. being individualistic

and China being collectivist), in order to determine if a difference in social

loafing was present between the two types of cultures. Earley formed groups

from both countries similar in demographics and in time spent with each other

(participants in each of the groups had known each other for three to five

weeks). Each group was tasked with completing various forms of paperwork

similar to work they would be required to do in their profession. The paperwork

was designed to take two to five minutes for each item, and the items were

turned in to an assistant when completed so that no one could judge their work

compared to others. Each participant was given 60 minutes to complete as many

items as possible and was separated into either the high-accountability group,

where they were told they needed to achieve a group goal, or a

low-accountability group, where they were told they were to achieve a goal

alone. They were also separated into high and low shared responsibility groups.

It was found that, consistent with other studies, highly individualistic people

performed more poorly on the task when there was high shared responsibility and

low accountability than when there was high accountability. The collectivists,

however, performed somewhat better on the task when high shared responsibility

was present, regardless of how accountable they were supposed to be as compared

to when they were working alone. This evidence suggests that collectivist

thinking reduces the social loafing effect. Further evidence from a similar

study showed the effect was related to the collectivist thinking rather than

nationality, as individualistic Chinese workers did indeed show a social

loafing effect.[11]

Causes

Diffusion of responsibility/Evaluation potential

As the number of people in the group or team increase, people tend to feel

deindividuation. This term defines both the dissociation from individual

achievement and the decrease of personal accountability, resulting in lower

exerted effort for individuals in collaborative environments. This phenomenon

can thus decrease overall group effectiveness because it is contagious and hard

to correct. Once identified by the group or team leader, it is their

responsibility to reassess and put into motion new rules and expectations for

everyone.

People could simply feel "lost in the crowd", so they feel that their effort

would not be rewarded even if they put it forth. This idea can also cause

people to feel as though they can simply "hide in the crowd" and avoid the

averse effects of not applying themselves.[8]

When enthusiasm for the overall goal or task is diminished, overall

contribution will drop. When one feels that their overall efforts are reduced

or unimportant, they will likely become social loafers.

Motivation

Social psychological literature has found that the level of motivation one has

to engage in an activity influences one s behavior in a group setting. This

finding, deemed the collective effort model by Karau and Williams (1993, 2001)

details that individuals who are more motivated are more likely to engage in

social facilitation (that is, to increase one s efforts when in the presence of

others) whereas those who are less motivated are more likely to engage in

social loafing.[12] Researchers have determined that two factors which

determine an individual s motivation, and subsequently whether or not the

individual will resort to social loafing versus social facilitation, include

the individual s expectations about attaining the goal and the perceived value

of the goal.

Thus, a person s attitude toward these two factors will influence his or her

motivation level and subsequent group behavior. Karau and Williams (1993, 2001)

found that motivation was highest when the individual believed that the goal

was easily attainable and very valuable. On the other hand, motivation was

lowest when the goal seemed impossible and not at all valuable.[12]

Unfortunately, the presence of a group can influence one s perception of these

two factors in a number of ways. For instance, working in a group may reduce or

increase one s expectancy of attaining a goal. That is, depending on the

qualities of the group members, an individual may find themselves in a group of

high achievers who work hard and are guaranteed success, whereas another may

equally find themselves in a group of lazy or distracted people, making success

seem unattainable. Therefore, the link between one s personal efforts and

success is not direct, as our success is influenced by the work of others.

Similarly, the value of the goal may be contingent on the group members. For

instance, if we must share the reaping of success with all other group members,

then the value of the goal is reduced compared to the value of the goal from an

individual perspective. Hence, the dynamic of the group is an important key in

determining a person s motivation and the likelihood of social loafing.[12]

Additional factors which have been found to influence the likelihood of social

loafing include one s gender, one s cultural background and the complexity of

the task.

Dispensability of effort

When a group member does not feel that his/her effort is justified in the

context of the overall group, the individual will be less willing to assert the

effort. If the group size is large, members can feel that their contribution

will not be worth much to the overall cause because so many other contributions

can or should occur. This leads people to not contribute as much or at all in

large groups as they might have in smaller groups.

One example is voting in the United States. Even though most people say that

voting is important, and a good practice for them to do, every year a

sub-optimal percentage of Americans turn up to vote, especially in presidential

elections (only 51% in the 2000 election).[13] One vote may feel very small in

a group of millions, so people may not think a vote is worth the time and

effort. If too many people think this way, there is a small percentage of voter

turnout. Some countries enforce compulsory voting to reduce this effect.

"Sucker" effect/Aversion

People feel that others in the group will leave them to do all the work while

they take the credit. Because people do not want to feel like the "sucker,"

they wait to see how much effort others will put into a group before they put

any in. If all the members try to avoid being the sucker, then everyone's

effort will be significantly less than it would be if all of them were working

as hard as they could.[14]

For example, in a workplace environment, the establishment of an absence

culture creates an attitude that all employees deserve to have a certain number

of days of absence, regardless of whether or not they are actually sick.

Therefore, if an employee has not used the maximum number of absence days, "he

may feel that he is carrying an unfair share of the workload". [4]

Attribution and equity/Matching of effort

Jackson and Harkins (1985) proposed that if someone feels that others in the

group are slacking or that others will slack, he will lower his effort to match

that of the others. This can occur whether it is apparent that the others are

slacking or if someone simply believes that the group is slacking.[1][15] For

example, in the Latane et al. study above, if a participant heard the others

making less noise than anticipated, he could have lowered his effort in an

attempt to equal that of the others, rather than aiming for the optimum.[8]

Submaximal goal setting

By setting a goal that is based on maximization, people may feel that there is

a set level that the group needs to be achieved. Because of this, they feel

that they can work less hard for the overall desired effect.

For example, in the Latane et al. clapping and shouting study, people who were

alone but told that they were part of a group screaming or clapping could have

thought that there was a set level of noise that experimenters were looking

for, and so assumed they could work less hard to achieve this level depending

on the size of the group.[8]

Real-life instances

1994 Black Hawk shootdown incident

Main article: 1994 Black Hawk shootdown incident

On April 14, 1994, two U.S. Air Force F-15 fighters accidentally shot down two

U.S. Army Black Hawk helicopters over Northern Iraq, killing all 26 soldiers on

board. The details of the incident have been analyzed by West Point Professor

Scott Snook in his book Friendly Fire.[16] In his summary of the fallacy of

social redundancy, Snook points to social loafing as a contributor to the

failure of the AWACS aircraft team to track the helicopters and prevent the

shootdown. Snook asserts that responsibility was "spread so thin by the laws of

social impact and confused authority relationships that no one felt compelled

to act".[16]

Social loafing and the workplace

According to Hwee Hoon Tan and Min-Li Tan, social loafing is an important area

of interest in order to understand group work.[17] While the opposite of social

loafing, "organizational citizenship behavior", can create significant

productivity increases, both of these behaviors can significantly impact the

performance of organizations. Social loafing is a behavior that organizations

want to eliminate. Understanding how and why people become social loafers is

critical to the effective functioning, competitiveness and effectiveness of an

organization.

There are certain examples of social loafing in the workplace that are

discussed by James larsen in his essay "Loafing on The Job." For example,

Construction men working vigorously on a construction site while some of their

working partners are lounging on rock walls or leaning on their shovels doing

nothing. another example is when you walk into a restaurant such as Mcdonalds

and find that some employees are lounging about while others are eagerly

awaiting your order. these scenarios all express the problems that social

loafing creates in a workplace and businesses would like to find a way to

counteract these trends.

Larsen mentions ways that a business could change its operations in order to

fight the negative effects of social loafing. For one, research has shown that

if each employee has their performance individually measured, they will put in

more effort than if it was not measured. Another person interested in the idea

of social loafing is Kenneth Price, from The University of Texas. Price

conducted a social loafing experiment in order to examine whether two key

factors that he suspected played a role in the way social loafing arose in work

groups. These two factors were dispensability and fairness. The experiment that

he conducted involved 514 people that were divided into 144 teams that were set

to meet for fourteen weeks. The projects assigned to these people were very

complex and called for diverse skills from many different individuals in order

to be fully completed. The experiments findings did in fact corroborate Price's

suspicions in the two factors of dispensability and fairness.

dispensability in a group is describes by Price as employees who join a work

group and quickly begin to measure up their skills with the people that they

are assigned to work with. If they feel that their skills are inferior to those

around them, people tend to sit back and let the other more skilled workers

carry the workload. Fairness in a group is when some group members feel that

their voice is not heard in decision making because of their ethnicity, gender

or other discriminatory factors. Instead of fighting for their voice to be

heard many group members will decide to loaf in these circumstances.

Online communities and groups

Research regarding social loafing online is currently relatively sparse, but is

growing.[3]

A 2008 study of 227 undergraduate and graduate students enrolled in web-enabled

courses at the Naval War College (NWC) and a public university found that

social loafing not only exists, but may also be prevalent in the online

learning classroom. Although only 2% of NWC and 8% of public university

students self-reported social loafing, 8% of NWC and 77% of public university

students indicated the perception of others engaging in social loafing.

Additional findings generally verify face-to-face social loafing findings from

previous studies. The researchers conclude that injustice in the distribution

of rewards increases social loafing, and suggest that self-perceived dominance

negatively affects individual participation in group activities.[3]

Social loafing, also known as "lurking", greatly affect the development and

growth of online communities. The term social loafing refers to the tendency

for individuals to expend less effort when working collectively than when

working individually.[1] This phenomenon is much like people s tendency to be

part of a group project, but rely heavily on just a few individuals to complete

the work. Generally, social loafers regularly follow the discussions and

content of online communities, but choose not to expand on posts or add to the

knowledge of the community.[18] Additionally, participation in online

communities is usually voluntary; therefore there is no guarantee that

community members will contribute to the knowledge of the website, discussion

forum, bulletin board, or other form of online engagement.

Lurkers are reported to constitute over 90% of several online groups.[1]

The main reason people choose not to contribute to online communities

surprisingly does not have to do with societal laziness, but in fact the

potential contributors belief that their entries will not be taken seriously or

given the credit that they deserve. When people assess the risks involved in

contributing to online communities, they generally avoid participation because

of the uncertainty of who the other contributors and readers are and the fear

of their work being undervalued.[18]

Age-related effects on participation

Although studies justify the notion that people often do not contribute to

online communities, some research shows that older adults are actually more

likely to participate in online communities than younger people because

different generations tend to use the internet differently. For example, "older

adults are more likely to seek health information, make purchases, and obtain

religious information, but less likely to watch videos, download music, play

games, and read blogs online".[19] This is perhaps due in part to the fact that

some online communities cater to older generations. The content of the website

often determines what age group will use or visit the site, and because many

forms of online communities appear on sites that focus their attention on older

adults, participation is generally higher. Additionally, the ease and

availability of operating the websites that host the online community may play

a role in the age group that is most likely to participate. For example, some

online communities geared toward older adults have simplified the design of

their sites in order to enhance their look and usability for older adults.[19]

Reducing social loafing

According to Dan J. Rothwell, it takes "the three Cs of motivation" to get a

group moving: collaboration, content, and choice.[20] Thus, the answer to

social loafing may be motivation. A competitive environment may not necessarily

get group members motivated.

Collaboration is a way to get everyone involved in the group by assigning each

member special, meaningful tasks.[21] It is a way for the group members to

share the knowledge and the tasks to be fulfilled unfailingly. For example, if

Sally and Paul were loafing because they were not given specific tasks, then

giving Paul the note taker duty and Sally the brainstorming duty will make them

feel essential to the group. Sally and Paul will be less likely to want to let

the group down, because they have specific obligations to complete.

Content identifies the importance of the individual's specific tasks within the

group. If group members see their role as that involved in completing a worthy

task, then they are more likely to fulfill it. For example, Sally may enjoy

brainstorming, as she knows that she will bring a lot to the group if she

fulfills this obligation. She feels that her obligation will be valued by the

group.

Choice gives the group members the opportunity to choose the task they want to

fulfill. Assigning roles in a group causes complaints and frustration. Allowing

group members the freedom to choose their role makes social loafing less

significant, and encourages the members to work together as a team.

Thompson stresses that ability and motivation are essential, but insufficient

for effective team functioning. A team must also coordinate the skills,

efforts, and actions of its members in order to effectively achieve its goal.

Thompson's recommendations can be separated into motivation strategies and

coordination strategies:[22]

Motivation strategies Coordination strategies

Increase identifiability

Promote involvement

Reward team members for performance

Strengthen team cohesion

Increase personal responsibility

Use team contracts

Provide team performance reviews and feedback

Using single-digit teams

Having an agenda

Training team members together

Spending more time practicing

Minimizing links in communication

Setting clear performance standards

Motivational strategies

Increase identifiability: Studies of social loafing suggest that people are

less productive when they are working with others, but social facilitation

studies have shown that people are more productive when others are present (at

least with an easy task). If individuals within a group know one another, feel

that their productivity or inputs are not identifiable, then social loafing is

likely to occur. Alternatively, if individuals are anonymous and therefore

unidentifiable, then social loafing may also be likely to occur.[23]

Minimize free riding: Free riding occurs when members do less than their share

of the work because others will make up for their slack. As others contribute

ideas, individuals may feel less motivated to work hard themselves. They see

their own contributions as less necessary or less likely to have much impact.

[24]To eliminate these effects, it is important to make group members feel that

their contributions are essential for the group s success. Additionally, it is

less likely for someone to free-ride if they are in a small group.[23]

Promote involvement: loafing is also less likely to occur when people are

involved with their work, and when they enjoy working with others in groups.

These are people who value both the experience of being part of a group, as

well as achieving results. Also, challenging and difficult tasks reduce social

loafing. Social loafing is also reduced when individuals are involved in group

work and their rewards are received as a team, rather than individually.[23]

Strengthen team cohesion: the extent to which group members identify with their

group also determines the amount of social loafing. This concept links with

social identity theory in that that difference between a hard-working group and

one that is loafing is the match between the group s tasks and its members

self definitions. When individuals derive their sense of self and identity from

their membership, social loafing is replaced by social laboring (members will

expand extra effort for their group).[23]

Set goals: groups that set clear, challenging goals outperform groups whose

members have lost sight of their objectives. The group's goals should be

relatively challenging, instead of being too easily accomplished. The

advantages of working in a group are often lost when a task is so easy that it

can be accomplished even when members of the group socially loaf. Thus, groups

should ensure to set their standards high, but not so high that the goals are

unattainable. Latham and Baldes (1975) assessed the practical significance of

Locke's theory of goal setting by conducting an experiment with truck drivers

who hauled logs from the woods to the mill. When the men were initially told to

do their best when loading the logs, they carried only about 60% of the weight

that they could legally haul. When the same drivers were later encouraged to

reach a goal of hauling 94% of the legal limit, they increased their efficiency

and met this specific goal. Thus, the results of this study show that

performance improved immediately upon the assignment of a specific, challenging

goal. Company cost accounting procedures indicated that this same increase in

performance without goal setting would have required an expenditure of a

quarter of a million dollars on the purchase of additional trucks alone. So

this method of goal setting is extremely effective.[25] Other research has

found that clear goals can stimulate a number of other performance-enhancing

processes, including increases in effort, better planning, more accurate

monitoring of the quality of the groups work, and even an increased commitment

to the group.[26]

Individual Assessment In order to reduce social loafing, a company can always

focus on assessing each members contribution rather than only examining the

teams accomplishments as a whole. It is statistically proven that social

loafers will tend to put in less effort because of the lack of no external or

internal assessment of their contributions. This leads to less self-awareness

in the group because the team together is the only body evaluated. (Curt. 2007)

Encouraging contributions in online communities

Piezon & Donaldson argue in a 2005 analysis that special attention should be

paid to the physical separation, social isolation, and temporal distance

associated with distance education courses, which may induce social loafing. In

terms of group size, they assert that there is no significant gain in small

groups larger than six unless the group is brainstorming, and that the optimal

group size may be five members. Suggestions that they have for online groups

include clarifying roles and responsibilities, providing performance data for

comparison with other groups, and mandating high levels of participation

consisting of attending group meetings, using the discussion board, and

participating in chats.[27]

In a 2010 analysis of online communities, Kraut and Resnick suggest several

ways to elicit contributions from users:[28]

Simply asking users, either implicitly through selective presentation of tasks

or explicitly through requests that play on the principles of persuasion

Changing the composition or activity of the group

Using a record-keeping system to reflect member contributions, in addition to

awarding privileges or more tangible awards

An example that the authors study is Wikipedia, which runs fundraising

campaigns that involve tens of thousands of people and raise millions of

dollars by employing large banner ads at the top of the page with deadlines,

specific amounts of money set as the goal, and lists of contributors.

Reduction in group projects

In 2008, Praveen Aggarwal and Connie O'Brien studied several hundred college

students assessing what factors can reduce social loafing during group

projects. From the results, they concluded that there were three factors that

reduce social loafing.[29]

Limiting the scope of the project: Instructors can reduce social loafing by

either dividing a big project into two or more smaller components or replacing

semester-long projects with a smaller project and some other graded work. Also,

breaking up a big project into smaller components can be beneficial.[29]

Smaller group size: Limiting the group size can make it harder for social

loafers to hide behind the shield of anonymity provided by a large group. In

smaller groups, each member will feel that their contribution will add greater

value.[29]

Peer evaluations: Peer evaluations send a signal to group members that there

will be consequences for non-participation. It has been found that as the

number of peer evaluations during a project go up, the incidence of social

loafing goes down.

http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Social_loafing