💾 Archived View for gmi.noulin.net › mobileNews › 2453.gmi captured on 2023-09-08 at 18:31:51. Gemini links have been rewritten to link to archived content

View Raw

More Information

⬅️ Previous capture (2023-01-29)

➡️ Next capture (2024-05-10)

-=-=-=-=-=-=-

The science of optical illusions

2010-10-18 05:22:08

Why do these two surfaces in the same shade of grey appear so different?

Optical illusions are more than just a bit of fun. Scientist Beau Lotto is

finding out what tricking the brain reveals about how our minds work. Here he

explains his findings.

Sight, hearing, taste, touch, smell. We believe what our senses tell us but

most of all we trust our eyes.

But our brains are extraordinarily powerful organs.

Without us realising it, they are instantly processing the information they

receive to make sense of the world around us.

And that has been crucial to our evolution.

Jungle scene

Take colour. Why do we need to see in colour? The next two images will show you

why.

Here you see a black-and-white version of a jungle scene. Try to find the

predator that's about to jump out at you. If it takes you more than a second,

you are dead. Why is it so difficult to find? Because you are only seeing the

surfaces according to the amount of light they reflect. Now click to the next

image...

... this time in colour. Now you'll probably see the panther immediately (in

the lower right corner).

Why is it so easy this time? The reason is because the image shows the surfaces

according to the quality of light they reflect (not just their intensity).

In other words, your brain has a lot more information to go on in making its

decisions.

So colour enables us to see a greater number of similarities and differences

between objects, which is necessary for survival.

What is amazing about what you have just done (finding the panther in the

coloured image) is that it is so easy for you.

But while it seems so easy, our best computers are hopeless at doing what you

have just done.

Understanding how we see is one of the main aims of brain science (called

neuroscience). And illusions hold the key to answering this question.

Brightness illusion

Below we have two physically identical squares. Not surprisingly, they also

look the same. Explaining vision would be easy if all we had to do is see the

image that falls onto the back of the eye (called the retinal image). But we

don't.

In fact we never see what our eyes see. That's because the eyes have very

little to do with what we see. This is good news: an image of the world is very

different from the world itself.

For instance, the retinal image has only two dimensions, whereas the world of

course has three. The retinal image is upside-down, but we see the world

right-side-up. So what happens if I change the context surrounding the squares,

but not the two squares themselves?

Toggle the image and the two identical squares now look different.

And yet all we've done is put them on different backgrounds. As a result, the

small square on the dark background looks lighter than the one on the light

background.

This is called the "brightness contrast illusion", which proves that context is

everything when it comes to what we see, even when seeing the simplest

qualities of the world, namely lightness.

But why is context everything?

Table and shadow illusion

Here we have two, smaller versions of an identical brightness contrast illusion

- one on the right and the other on the left.

In both cases the tiles on the dark backgrounds look lighter than the tiles on

the light backgrounds. So far so good. Now let's see if we can change the

strength of these two illusions by changing the overall scene.

Notice that the illusion on the left is now much stronger. In other words, the

tile in shadow under the table looks much brighter because the brain thinks it

is in shade. The tile to the right looks as if it is under bright light so the

brain assumes it is darker and tells us so.

On the other hand the illusion on the right is now much weaker. In other words,

the two tiles, one on a black stripe the other on a light stripe, look nearly

identical because the brain is interpreting them as two similarly reflective

tiles under a shared light source.

This shows that we see illusions because the brain doesn't actually want to see

the image on your eye, but to see the meaning of that image and here it finds

that in the context of the table and the light from the window. And that

meaning - and this is really important - is created from experience.

Cube illusion

Here we have two tiles that are identical in their colour. But what happens if

we change their context in a specific way? Now if we're right that what we see

is the meaning of an image, then it we should be able to create a really strong

illusion by making the meaning of the two tiles very different indeed.

In their new context, the two physically identical tiles do indeed now look

very different.

Why? The information in the image strongly suggests that the dark brown tile on

the top now means a poorly reflective surface under bright light, whereas the

bright orange one at the side means a highly reflective surface in shadow.

So you see them differently because your brain thinks they have a different

meaning - given the rest of the information in the scene.

Table illusion

What's true for seeing colour is also true for seeing form and shape. In fact

it's true about everything we see. When you look at this image, you are aware

of two very differently sized tables.

The one of the left seems a lot longer and thinner than the one on the right.

What if I tell you that the the red table is simply the green table on its

side, in other words that the dimensions of the two table tops is identical.

It is actually true. The only real difference between the tables is the angles

at their corners (other than their colour, which is irrelevant in this case).

The two red and green lines are the same length. The length of the red table is

the same as the width of the green table and vice versa.

So why do they look so different? Because your brain takes the image on the

retina and creates what it sees according to what the information would have

meant in the brain's past experience of interacting with the world.

In this case the angles suggest depth and perspective and the brain believes

the green table is longer than it is while the red table appears squarer.

The beautiful thing about illusions is they make us realise things are never

what they seem, and that our experiences of the world shape our understanding

of it.

--- Mobile internet site for reading on mobile phones, smartphones, small

screens and slow internet connections. ---http://mpggalaxy.mine.bz/www/BB/

mobile_news/threads/index_last.html

Posted: 2010899@484.45

━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━━

stranger

I am truly amazed! 'Seeing is believing' is (or was) my foremost mantra. Now I

am confused, not to say somewhat worried. Am I only seeing what my brain tells

me is there - and not what is actually there?!

Chris Ferguson, Wisbech, England

I found the article very iintersting. About 50 years ago - I'm now 80 - I did

an entire exibit on opital illussions at the library in Wilton, Connecticut

inclcuding a wire cube that the viewer thougt was a square. There were also

depth perception pieces which where flat but seen as 3-dimensianal. In all

there were about 49 exhibits and lots of delighted visitors.

Prof Haji Rudin Salinger, Putrajaya, Malaysia

This suggests to me that a large part of the percieved world and therefore

constructed from experience - I wonder what the world really looks like? I

suppose we have all seen the world in this way when we were babies - but is it

possible to recreate this? How do we achieve "real seeing" as adults?

David Nixon, Hebden Bridge

Very interesting and informative - if you are colour blind how much would this

affect what you see

Barbara, Bamako, Mali

I actually Photoshopped the image of the cubes because I didn't believe it, but

yeah, same colour!

Michael , Belfast

There's a painting on my wall. Even though I know it's a flat canvas splodged

with pigment, it looks like a wonderful sea view with sand, water, birds and

the rest of it. Is this an optical illusion? No, because I'm not misjudging

anything! I'm correctly judging the image be a representation of the sea. I'm

inclined to say exactly the same thing about the "optical illusions" in this

article. For instance, we're not making a mistake when we judge the squares on

the Rubik's cube to be differently coloured. We are correctly taking the flat

image to represent a 3D cube on which the squares really are different colours.

Jonathan Birch, Cambridge, UK

A truly beautiful presentation, and lucidly explained. I admit that in a few

cases I took measurements to check if the images in the sets weren't "enhanced"

to magnify the illusions: they weren't.

Jaroslaw, Cairo, Egypt

Very interesting article but the last example with the two tables is a bad

drawing which is why the brain gets the wrong idea rather than being due to the

brains intrepretation of perspective. Assuming it's a table and therefore has

right angles at the legs then using the legs and the square floor tiles as a

measure, the green table is 5 tiles along it's longest side between the legs

and the red table is less than 4. The same kind of difference applies to the

widths. I agree that the table tops are the same width and length give or take

a bit but the brains interpretation is confused by where we see the distance

between the legs on the floor which are not drawn as they should be so it's a

trick drawing rather than the brains interpretation of perspective.

Paul Orrock, London, UK