💾 Archived View for gmi.noulin.net › mobileNews › 3728.gmi captured on 2023-06-16 at 19:17:48. Gemini links have been rewritten to link to archived content
⬅️ Previous capture (2023-01-29)
-=-=-=-=-=-=-
2012-01-12 07:08:51
It's more than a week into 2012, and the resolutions which seemed so reasonable
at the end of December may now have already fallen by the wayside.
For those looking to stay on the straight and narrow, outside intervention in
the form of iPhone apps and websites could help.
These sites apply the theory of behavioural economics to personal behaviour
change.
In other words, they fine you if you won't get your lazy self to the gym or
toss those disgusting cigarettes once and for all - and in some cases, pay you
if you do.
The programs, StickK, GymPact and 21 Habit are all based on research showing
that people are more likely to meet their commitments if the stakes are high.
Start Quote
In a way, you are outsourcing your self-control to some of these gadgets.
Shahram Heshmat, professor of public health
StickK asks users to make a "commitment contract" regarding desired behaviour,
whether it's to make the bed every morning, spend more time with family, or
work out three times a week.
Users can then establish stakes for not meeting that commitment and appoint a
referee to keep them honest.
The stakes need not be monetary, but if they are, the website charges the
user's card when they fall short.
GymPact focuses on just gym habits: via the website or app, users set a goal
for weekly gym attendance and decide what to wager on achieving it.
If they do succeed, verified by checking into a fitness centre via GPS, they're
paid. If not, they pay the site, which then redistributes the money to other
gym-goers.
With 21 Habit, users make a three-week pact to do something once a day, be it
run five miles, cease nail-chewing, write in a journal, and give the site $21
( 13.68).
For the next 21 days, users self-report whether they made their goal. For every
day they do, they get a dollar back. For every day they don't, their dollar
goes to charity.
Cost v benefits
The idea of using a carrot or a stick to influence healthy choices is not a new
one.
As American companies have found themselves exposed to rising healthcare costs
incurred by their workers, the study of behavioural economics took on more
urgency.
"Companies are seeing healthcare costs go up, and they're looking for some way
to bend that curve," says Mark Stehr, associate professor of economics at the
LeBow college of business at Drexel University in Philadelphia.
As a result, numerous studies have looked at what makes people choose healthy
behaviour, and overwhelming evidence shows that rewards and incentives can keep
people engaged.
Man at the gym on a laptop Can logging in keep you working out?
"Biologically we are all more sensitive to immediate gains and losses, or
costs, then we are to more delayed consequence," says Stephen T Higgins,
professor of psychiatry and psychology at the University of Vermont.
"When you're on your couch and want to exercise, the thing you are most
sensitive to is immediate costs. The benefit is delayed."
In other words, the desire to have a bikini body by June doesn't seem worth the
immediate cost of having to trudge through the cold to sweat for an hour,
especially when a Downton Abbey television marathon beckons.
By adding an immediate financial incentive, or making the cost of not achieving
your goal immediately more painful, sticking to a healthy behaviour can become
easier.
Technological boost
While studies have shown the power of money to motivate behaviour, the
application of this information had mostly been available to participants in
university-backed studies or incentive programmes sponsored by an employer.
Sites like StickK, 21Habit, and GymPact, however, harness the same tools for
individual use.
StickK, which has been available for consumer use since 2008, has a version of
the programme dedicated for corporate use. It also makes the software available
to researchers studying incentives.
Researchers who study behavioural economics say marrying this philosophy with
technology could be an excellent fit.
Most people experience two very distinct attitudes when trying to achieve a
goal: the enthusiastic person who packs a sports bag in the morning fully
intending to hit the gym after work, becomes the demotivated workout-dodger who
trudges through the door 10 hours later.
The power of misanthropy
When it comes time to designate where money from broken commitments is spent,
StickK offers users an unusual outlet.
Users can designate that money goes to a friend or a charity they support.
Or, they can send it straight to a cause they abhor.
Called "Anti-charity", StickK offers to send money to polarising organisations
that tend to inspire passion among the populace.
So if an anti-gun advocate needs extra motivation to eat more vegetables, she
can specify that any fees she incurs by not doing so go straight to a
gun-rights charity.
The desire to not support a cause one disagrees with has shown to be a strong
motivating force.
Jordan Goldberg, one of the founders of StickK, says those who choose
anti-charities have an almost 80% success rate, the highest of all their users.
An app that encourages users to keep their promises, even when they're at a
weak point, can help reinforce the first, more energetic attitude.
"Here I am, I'm so motivated, but at night when I come home I'm so tired," says
Shahram Heshmat, professor of public health at the University of Illinois in
Springfield.
"If there's a voice that reminds me that this is a vulnerable time, it can help
maintain motivation.
"In a way, you are outsourcing your self-control to some of these gadgets."
And because the motivation increases the more quickly rewards are delivered, an
app or website is well-suited for aiding this approach, says Prof Higgins.
"These apps can be very important because they reward behaviour right away," he
says.
There are drawbacks, mainly that the actions are self-reported. But people who
enter into these kinds of commitments usually want to change, and thus take the
process seriously, says Mr Stehr.
In his research, his team offered participants the chance to sign a commitment
contract after a study on incentivising gym habits ended.
While only 15% of the group decided to pursue commitment contracts, two-thirds
of them were successful in reaching their goals.
But what happens when the incentive stops?
While these programmes have shown definite benefits in the short term, "long
term is really the question," says Mr Stehr, who notes that there has not been
a lot of long-term investigation into the after-effects of such incentive
programmes.
What studies do exist mostly deal with incentives as a way to curb drug abuse
and addiction, and in those cases, there's "overwhelming evidence" that
incentive programmes help even after the incentive stop, says Dr Higgins.
When it comes to health and fitness, "those studies that are available show you
can improve weight loss and sustain the gains following the discontinuation of
the incentive".
On the other hand, he says, if an incentive programme is working, "Who says we
have to discontinue?"