đŸ Archived View for library.inu.red âș file âș solidarity-federation-testosterone-not-guilty.gmi captured on 2023-01-29 at 14:00:04. Gemini links have been rewritten to link to archived content
âĄïž Next capture (2024-06-20)
-=-=-=-=-=-=-
Title: Testosterone not guilty Author: Solidarity Federation Date: Autumn 1998 Language: en Topics: machismo, Direct Action Magazine, biology Source: Retrieved on June 20, 2005 from https://web.archive.org/web/20050620082329/http://www.directa.force9.co.uk/archive/da8-features.htm Notes: Published in Direct Action #8 â Autumn 1998.
Aggressive loutish lads are often considered to be âtestosterone
fuelledâ. More testosterone means increased, unfocused aggression; less
of it means calm and controlled behaviour. Or does it?
The link between aggressive behaviour and the group of hormones commonly
referred to as testosterone is more tenuous and certainly more complex
than many scientists would have you think.
Undoubtedly, there does appear to be a link between aggression and
testosterone, and indeed, if the source of the latter is removed (say by
castration), levels of the former are often seen to drop. But increase
levels, and initially there is no observable change. In fact, it takes a
massive increase to more than double normal levels to effect any
noticeable response.
Most importantly, even when aggression levels are increased, it is not
random and flying out wildly, but channelled down the socially
prescribed paths that are available. In a hierarchical primate society,
a male primate with suddenly massively increased levels of testosterone
coursing through its body would not go on a random attack, it would
still treat higher ranking primates with due respect, but would become a
complete sod to lower ranking primates.
Basically, testosterone facilitates increased levels of brain activity,
but not that associated with aggressive behaviour. The cause of
aggression is not simply the presence of testosterone, but its
interaction with other biological processes, and particularly, the
social environment.
In spotted hyenas in Kenya, females apparently have a lot more of a
testosterone related hormone than males. Females are larger, with
greater musculature, and tend to be socially dominant. In a colony that
has been transplanted to California, the physically identical females
have similarly high levels of the hormone and are similarly larger and
more muscular than their male counterparts. However, the level of social
domination has been considerably delayed in the captive, controlled,
California colony. A large element of the learnt âwildâ behaviour was
lost.
There are clear signs, then, that there is a balance between the
environment and biology. Certainly, it is not a straightforward case of
biological determinism (the idea that âphysical biology explains allâ).
Dodgy scientists, money grabbers and politicians can be relied on to
bend the truth to suit their own perverse ends. But however much
âsocio-biologyâ, âneurologyâ and âgeneticsâ research is done, there is
little chance of a fresh outbreak of the biological determinist picture
they try to paint.
In reality, biology (through the existence of life) provides potential,
and the environment shapes this potential. Aggressive behaviour is
shaped by a flawed social system, such as this one we live in. Creating
a better environment, physical and social, is the only way to
fundamentally alter this cycle of aggression. And by the way, you only
get research into ethically dubious areas when you live in an ethically
dubious society.