💾 Archived View for gmi.noulin.net › mobileNews › 5282.gmi captured on 2022-07-16 at 15:06:54. Gemini links have been rewritten to link to archived content

View Raw

More Information

⬅️ Previous capture (2021-12-03)

➡️ Next capture (2023-01-29)

-=-=-=-=-=-=-

Why can t you tickle yourself?

David Robson

It s almost impossible to get a laugh by self-tickling, says David Robson, and

the reason why tells us surprising things about the brain and consciousness.

If you want to probe some of the great mysteries of the human mind, all you

need is a duster and your feet. Sit back, take your shoes and socks off, and

gently stroke its feathers against your sole. Now ask a friend, parent or child

to do the same for you. If you are like most people, you will be left

stony-faced by one, but convulsed in a pleasurable agony by the other. How

come?

While asleep, people tried to get dream characters to tickle them - that too

failed Even in dreams, the brain stops self-tickling

Once the domain of childhood curiosity, the question of why we can t tickle

ourselves is exciting neuroscientists. It leads to these bigger questions of

consciousness and self-awareness, who we are, says George Van Doorn at Monash

University in Australia. For this reason, they are now going to some often

extreme lengths to overcome the brain s barriers and to get people to tickle

themselves in the lab.

To understand their interest, consider this: every time your body moves, it

creates potentially confusing sensations that could lead you astray in all

kinds of ways. Just imagine the chaos if you assumed that someone was fondling

or attacking you, every time one of your hands brushed your leg, for example.

Being able to differentiate between your movements, and the actions of other

people, is therefore a central part of our sense of self and agency aspects

of the psyche that even the most sophisticated robots can t replicate. And

examining these kinds of traits, you want to find an example that is easily

replicated in the lab. Tickling is a nice example because the contrast between

ticklish sensations produced by others and the inability to tickle oneself is

so clear, says Jennifer Windt at the Johannes Gutenberg University of Mainz in

Germany.

Sarah-Jayne Blakemore, at University College London, was one of the first to

investigate the way the brain makes these lightning-fast decisions about the

self and others. She scanned subjects brains as her colleagues tickled the

palms of their hands, and when they attempted to do so themselves. From the

resulting brain activity, she concluded that whenever we move our limbs, the

brain s cerebellum produces precise predictions of the body s movements, and

then sends a second shadow signal that damps down activity in the somatosensory

cortex where tactile feelings are processed. The result is that when we

tickle ourselves, we don t feel the sensations with the same intensity as if

they had come from someone else, and so we remain calm rather than writhing

with that familiar mix of discomfort and pleasure that comes when someone else

tickles us.

If that was true, she suspected that there could be ways to fool the process,

and allow people to tickle themselves. So she designed a machine that allowed

her subjects to move a stick that gently stroked a piece of foam over their

palm sometimes instantaneously, at others with a delay of up to 200

milliseconds. It turned out that the greater the delay, the more ticklish the

foam felt, perhaps because the cerebellum s predictions no longer matched what

the person was actually feeling.

Why do we laugh when tickled? The Headsqueeze team explains in the video above

Since Blakemore s ground-breaking studies, many others have tried to find ways

to find ways to trick the brain into tickling itself. Controlling someone s

foot movements with magnetic brain stimulation, so that their hand tickled

their foot against their will, seems to do the trick. But it is one of the few

experiments to succeed others have produced puzzling results.

Van Doorn, for example, tried to give his subjects an out-of-body experience

before tickling them. The set-up is relatively simple: the participant is

fitted with video goggles that allow them to see from the eyes of the

experimenter, who is sitting in front of them. By synchronising their

movements, they slowly begin to feel like the experimenter s body is their own.

(For more information on how to swap your body with someone else, read our

feature.)

In the midst of the illusion, the participants then had to move a lever that

would tickle both bodies at the same time. With the subject confused about

which body they were inhabiting, Van Doorn assumed that they would feel the

full force of the tickle but they were largely unmoved by the experience. No

matter if you swap bodies with someone else you can t tickle yourself with

your own movements, says Van Doorn.

You can t even tickle yourself in your dreams. Windt recently performed a dream

experiment that sounds like it came straight out of the movie Inception. She

recruited a team of expert lucid dreamers people who know they are dreaming,

and can control the actions of their dreams to try it out, but they couldn t.

The subjects also tried to get other dream characters to tickle them; that too

failed, sometimes because the other characters simply refused to be of service.

Several of our subjects experienced problems with, let s say, dream character

compliance, says Windt.

If all that seems a little esoteric, there could be practical reasons for

picking apart the neural processes behind self-tickling. It s interesting that

people with schizophrenia can tickle themselves and we think that s associated

with things like delusional and alien control of limbs, says Van Doorn

perhaps because of a more general problem with identifying the origins of their

movements. So attempts to break down that process in healthy people could,

eventually, shed some light on the way it malfunctions during periods of mental

illness.

Self-tickling could even improve artificial intelligence, says Robert Provine,

at the University of Maryland, Baltimore. Your inability to tickle yourself

suggests neurologically based definitions of self and other, he writes in a

playful essay. Developing a similar machine algorithm may lead to ticklish

robots whose performance is enhanced by their capacity to distinguish touching

from being touched, and, provocatively, may provide a computationally based

construct of machine personhood.

If so, a feather duster could provide a delightfully bizarre alternative to the

famous Turing test for artificial intelligence in years to come: just aim for

its extremities and see if it laughs.