💾 Archived View for gmi.noulin.net › mobileNews › 5282.gmi captured on 2022-07-16 at 15:06:54. Gemini links have been rewritten to link to archived content
⬅️ Previous capture (2021-12-03)
-=-=-=-=-=-=-
David Robson
It s almost impossible to get a laugh by self-tickling, says David Robson, and
the reason why tells us surprising things about the brain and consciousness.
If you want to probe some of the great mysteries of the human mind, all you
need is a duster and your feet. Sit back, take your shoes and socks off, and
gently stroke its feathers against your sole. Now ask a friend, parent or child
to do the same for you. If you are like most people, you will be left
stony-faced by one, but convulsed in a pleasurable agony by the other. How
come?
While asleep, people tried to get dream characters to tickle them - that too
failed Even in dreams, the brain stops self-tickling
Once the domain of childhood curiosity, the question of why we can t tickle
ourselves is exciting neuroscientists. It leads to these bigger questions of
consciousness and self-awareness, who we are, says George Van Doorn at Monash
University in Australia. For this reason, they are now going to some often
extreme lengths to overcome the brain s barriers and to get people to tickle
themselves in the lab.
To understand their interest, consider this: every time your body moves, it
creates potentially confusing sensations that could lead you astray in all
kinds of ways. Just imagine the chaos if you assumed that someone was fondling
or attacking you, every time one of your hands brushed your leg, for example.
Being able to differentiate between your movements, and the actions of other
people, is therefore a central part of our sense of self and agency aspects
of the psyche that even the most sophisticated robots can t replicate. And
examining these kinds of traits, you want to find an example that is easily
replicated in the lab. Tickling is a nice example because the contrast between
ticklish sensations produced by others and the inability to tickle oneself is
so clear, says Jennifer Windt at the Johannes Gutenberg University of Mainz in
Germany.
Sarah-Jayne Blakemore, at University College London, was one of the first to
investigate the way the brain makes these lightning-fast decisions about the
self and others. She scanned subjects brains as her colleagues tickled the
palms of their hands, and when they attempted to do so themselves. From the
resulting brain activity, she concluded that whenever we move our limbs, the
brain s cerebellum produces precise predictions of the body s movements, and
then sends a second shadow signal that damps down activity in the somatosensory
cortex where tactile feelings are processed. The result is that when we
tickle ourselves, we don t feel the sensations with the same intensity as if
they had come from someone else, and so we remain calm rather than writhing
with that familiar mix of discomfort and pleasure that comes when someone else
tickles us.
If that was true, she suspected that there could be ways to fool the process,
and allow people to tickle themselves. So she designed a machine that allowed
her subjects to move a stick that gently stroked a piece of foam over their
palm sometimes instantaneously, at others with a delay of up to 200
milliseconds. It turned out that the greater the delay, the more ticklish the
foam felt, perhaps because the cerebellum s predictions no longer matched what
the person was actually feeling.
Why do we laugh when tickled? The Headsqueeze team explains in the video above
Since Blakemore s ground-breaking studies, many others have tried to find ways
to find ways to trick the brain into tickling itself. Controlling someone s
foot movements with magnetic brain stimulation, so that their hand tickled
their foot against their will, seems to do the trick. But it is one of the few
experiments to succeed others have produced puzzling results.
Van Doorn, for example, tried to give his subjects an out-of-body experience
before tickling them. The set-up is relatively simple: the participant is
fitted with video goggles that allow them to see from the eyes of the
experimenter, who is sitting in front of them. By synchronising their
movements, they slowly begin to feel like the experimenter s body is their own.
(For more information on how to swap your body with someone else, read our
feature.)
In the midst of the illusion, the participants then had to move a lever that
would tickle both bodies at the same time. With the subject confused about
which body they were inhabiting, Van Doorn assumed that they would feel the
full force of the tickle but they were largely unmoved by the experience. No
matter if you swap bodies with someone else you can t tickle yourself with
your own movements, says Van Doorn.
You can t even tickle yourself in your dreams. Windt recently performed a dream
experiment that sounds like it came straight out of the movie Inception. She
recruited a team of expert lucid dreamers people who know they are dreaming,
and can control the actions of their dreams to try it out, but they couldn t.
The subjects also tried to get other dream characters to tickle them; that too
failed, sometimes because the other characters simply refused to be of service.
Several of our subjects experienced problems with, let s say, dream character
compliance, says Windt.
If all that seems a little esoteric, there could be practical reasons for
picking apart the neural processes behind self-tickling. It s interesting that
people with schizophrenia can tickle themselves and we think that s associated
with things like delusional and alien control of limbs, says Van Doorn
perhaps because of a more general problem with identifying the origins of their
movements. So attempts to break down that process in healthy people could,
eventually, shed some light on the way it malfunctions during periods of mental
illness.
Self-tickling could even improve artificial intelligence, says Robert Provine,
at the University of Maryland, Baltimore. Your inability to tickle yourself
suggests neurologically based definitions of self and other, he writes in a
playful essay. Developing a similar machine algorithm may lead to ticklish
robots whose performance is enhanced by their capacity to distinguish touching
from being touched, and, provocatively, may provide a computationally based
construct of machine personhood.
If so, a feather duster could provide a delightfully bizarre alternative to the
famous Turing test for artificial intelligence in years to come: just aim for
its extremities and see if it laughs.